奶昔直播官方版-奶昔直播直播视频在线观看免费版下载-奶昔直播安卓版本免费安装

育路教育網(wǎng),權(quán)威招生服務(wù)平臺(tái)
新東方在線

考研閱讀精選:膚色不同,結(jié)果相異

來(lái)源:新東方在線 時(shí)間:2011-10-24 09:18:22

『在美國(guó)科學(xué)界,種族和膚色是影響科研人員職稱評(píng)定、獲取科研經(jīng)費(fèi)的重要因素,而這種歧視會(huì)造成人才的極大浪費(fèi)�!�

  A black and white answer

  膚色不同,結(jié)果相異

  August 20th 2011 | from The Economist

  

  YOU might expect that science, particularly American science, would be colour-blind. Though fewer people from some of the country’s ethnic minorities are scientists than the proportions of those minorities in the population suggest should be the case, once someone has got bench space in a laboratory, he might reasonably expect to be treated on merit and nothing else.

  Unfortunately, a study just published in Science by Donna Ginther of the University of Kansas suggests that is not true. Dr Ginther, who was working on behalf of America’s National Institutes of Health (NIH), looked at the pattern of research grants awarded by the NIH and found that race matters a lot. Moreover, it is not just a question of white supremacy. Asian and Hispanic scientists do just as well as white ones. Black scientists, however, do badly.

  Dr Ginther and her colleagues analysed grants awarded by the NIH between 2000 and 2006, and correlated this information with the self-reported race of more than 40,000 applicants. Their results show that the chance of a black scientist receiving a grant was 17%. For Asians, Hispanics and whites the number was between 26% and 29%. Even when these figures were adjusted to take into account applicants’ prior education, awards, employment history and publications, a gap of ten percentage points remained.

  This bias appears to arise in the NIH’s peer-review mechanism. Each application is reviewed by a panel of experts. These panels assign scores to about half the applications they receive. Scored applications are then considered for grants by the various institutes that make up the NIH. The race of the applicant is not divulged to the panel. However, Dr Ginther found that applications from black scientists were less likely to be awarded a score than those from similarly qualified scientists of other races, and when they were awarded a score, that score was lower than the scores given to applicants of other races.

  One possible explanation is that review panels are inferring applicants’ ethnic origins from their names, or the institutions they attended as students. The reviewers may then be awarding less merit to those from people with “black-sounding” names, or who were educated at universities whose students are predominantly black. Indeed, a similar bias has been found in those recruiting for jobs in the commercial world. One well-known study, published in 2003 by researchers at the Massachusetts Institute of Technology and the University of Chicago, found that fictitious CVs with stereotypically white names elicited 50% more offers of interviews than did CVs with black names, even when the applicants’ stated qualifications were identical.

  Another possible explanation is social networking. It is in the nature of groups of experts to know both each other and each other’s most promising acolytes. Applicants outside this charmed circle might have less chance of favourable consideration. If the charmed circle itself were racially unrepresentative (if professors unconsciously preferred graduate students of their own race, for example), those excluded from the network because their racial group was under-represented in the first place would find it harder to break in.

  Though Dr Ginther’s results are troubling, it is to the NIH’s credit that it has published her findings. The agency is also starting a programme intended to alter the composition of the review panels, and—appropriately for a scientific body—will conduct experiments to see whether excising potential racial cues from applications changes outcomes. Other agencies, should pay strict attention to all this, and ask themselves if they, too, are failing people of particular races. Such discrimination is not only disgraceful, but also a stupid waste of talent.(598 words)

結(jié)束

特別聲明:①凡本網(wǎng)注明稿件來(lái)源為"原創(chuàng)"的,轉(zhuǎn)載必須注明"稿件來(lái)源:育路網(wǎng)",違者將依法追究責(zé)任;

②部分稿件來(lái)源于網(wǎng)絡(luò),如有侵權(quán),請(qǐng)聯(lián)系我們溝通解決。

有用

25人覺得有用

閱讀全文

2019考研VIP資料免費(fèi)領(lǐng)取

【隱私保障】

育路為您提供專業(yè)解答

相關(guān)文章推薦

24

2011.10

考研閱讀精選:知識(shí)產(chǎn)權(quán)-對(duì)專利制度對(duì)癥下藥

『本文主要介紹了美國(guó)專利制度需要改革的原因及如何進(jìn)行改革�!弧 ntellectual property:Patent m......

24

2011.10

考研閱讀精選:付費(fèi)電視的未來(lái)-推陳出新

『作為媒體行業(yè)中重要的贏利模式以及產(chǎn)出精彩節(jié)目的主要來(lái)源,付費(fèi)電視模式日漸衰落。』  The futur......

24

2011.10

海文考研英語(yǔ)�?及啵ňW(wǎng)授班)正在報(bào)名中

  海文考研英語(yǔ)模考班(網(wǎng)授)  開班日期:2011年11月中下旬  上課提示:下午:13:30  優(yōu)惠價(jià):......

24

2011.10

考研閱讀精選:如何調(diào)整美國(guó)的數(shù)學(xué)教育

『如今,美國(guó)的數(shù)學(xué)教育引起了國(guó)人的廣泛擔(dān)憂。數(shù)學(xué)源于實(shí)際生活應(yīng)用,但美國(guó)現(xiàn)行的數(shù)學(xué)課程已與生活脫......

24

2011.10

海文考研英語(yǔ)沖刺串講班 名師面授 報(bào)名從速

  海文考研英語(yǔ)沖刺串講班  開班日期:2011-11-26 ~ 2011-11-27  優(yōu)惠價(jià):¥350元  課時(shí):16......

24

2011.10

海文考研英語(yǔ)超級(jí)作文模板班正在搶報(bào)中

  海文考研英語(yǔ)超級(jí)作文模板班  開班日期:2011年11月底  上課提示:共4次課,(一上午算一次課)......

您可能感興趣
為什么要報(bào)考研輔導(dǎo)班? 如何選擇考研輔導(dǎo)班? 考研輔導(dǎo)班哪個(gè)好? 哪些北京考研輔導(dǎo)班靠譜? 2019考研輔導(dǎo)班大全